Baby talk

This article is about speech directed at babies. For speech-like sounds produced by babies, see babbling. For other uses, see Baby talk (disambiguation).
"Whoopsie daisy" redirects here. For the song by Terri Walker, see L.O.V.E (album).

Baby talk, also referred to as caretaker speech, infant-directed speech (IDS), child-directed speech (CDS) or motherese,[1][2][3][4][5] is usually delivered with a "cooing" pattern of intonation different from that of normal adult speech: high in pitch, with many glissando variations that are more pronounced than those of normal speech.[6] It frequently displays hyperarticulation, which is an increase in the distances between peripheral vowels (such as [i], [u], and [a]). Baby talk is also characterized by the shortening and simplifying of words. Baby talk is similar to what is used by people when talking to their pets (pet-directed speech). When adults talk to each other using baby talk it is generally to either show affection by emulating the fondness shown by adults for children, or as a form of bullying or condescension as children are much less cognitively developed than adults, implying that the adult receiving the baby talk is less intelligent than the adult talking to them.

Terminology

Purpose and implications

Use with infants

Studies have shown that from birth, infants prefer to listen to infant-directed speech, which is more effective than regular speech in getting and holding an infant's attention.[9] Some researchers, including Rima Shore,[10] believe that baby talk is an important part of the emotional bonding process between the parents and their child that helps the infants learn the language. Other researchers from Carnegie Mellon University and the University of Wisconsin confirm that using basic “baby talk” helps babies pick up words faster than usual.[11] Infants pay more attention when parents use infant-directed language, which has a slower and more repetitive tone than used in regular conversation. This child-directed speech has also been shown in languages other than English.[12]

Colwyn Trevarthen studied babies and their mothers, observing the communication and subtle movements between them. He has links to music therapy with other theorists.[13]

There are numerous purposes and benefits of infant directed speech, including positive effects on the early development of infants and children, and aid in the ability of infants to bond with their caregivers. In addition, infants begin the process of speech and language acquisition and development through infant directed speech.[14] Infant directed speech may also contribute to the modulation of infant attention, assist infants in determining relevant syntactic qualities including phonetic boundaries, and convey positive emotion to infants. The more expressive infant directed speech is, the more likely infants are to respond to this method of communication by adults.[15]

Perhaps the most important benefit of infant directed speech is what it provides to the development of language for infants and children. It has been found that the children who learn the fastest are those who receive the most acknowledgement and encouragement of what they say, who are given time and attention to speak and share, and who are questioned.[16] For instance, it has been found that six-month-olds can discriminate between medial position syllables in words with multiple syllables when infant directed speech is used. Therefore, infant directed speech is a powerful tool in providing a base for language acquisition. Infants are able to apply principles of this practice to larger words and sentences as they learn to process language.[17]

Infant directed speech also aids infants in selecting appropriate social partners. Although infants have a range of social cues available to them regarding who will provide adequate care, infant directed speech serves as an additional indicator as to which caregivers will provide developmental support. When adults engage in infant directed speech with infants they are ultimately providing them with positive emotion and attention, signaling to infants that they are valued.[18]

Infant directed speech can also serve as a priming tool for infants to notice the faces of their caregivers. Due to the nature of infant directed speech, infants are more sensitive to the pitch and emphasized qualities of this method. Therefore, when caregivers utilize infant directed speech they are expanding the possibility for their infants to notice and process their facial expressions. This effect could in part be due to the fact that infants associate infant directed speech with positive facial expressions such as smiling. Infants may be more likely to respond to infant directed speech if they expect to receive a positive response from their caregiver. Regardless, infant directed speech serves as a tool to aid infants in responding to facial expressions from caregivers, which in turn stimulates both language and social development.[19]

Research suggests that infant directed speech promotes the processing of word forms and allows infants to remember words when asked to recall them in the future. As words are repeated through infant directed speech, infants begin to create mental representations of each word. As a result, infants who experience infant directed speech are able to recall words more effectively than infants who do not.[20]

Research has shown that infants can pick up on the vocal cues of infant directed speech and will often pattern their babbling after it.[21]

The implications of the lack of the use of infant directed speech are equally as important to recognize. Research suggests that children of depressed mothers who do not regularly use infant directed speech display delayed language development. Even when depressed mothers provide their infants with positive faces, infants do not respond to their attempts at infant directed speech, and in turn do not benefit from this important method of language acquisition. Infants are unable to create the link between speech and visual face movements in situations such as these. When fathers who are not depressed are able to provide the stimulation of infant directed speech for their children, infants respond well and are able to compensate from the deficit left by their mothers. Unfortunately, it is likely that if infants do not receive adequate infant directed speech from their mothers they will generalize this response to all females. This too can inhibit language and speech development. Therefore, this deficit can be especially harmful to infants with depressed mothers and little contact with male caregivers.[22] Socioeconomic status has been found to influence the development of vocabulary and language skills. Lower-status groups tend to be behind the development of children in higher-status families. This finding is thought to be due to the amount of time parents spend with the child and the ways they interact; mothers from higher-status groups are found to say more to their children, use more variety, and speak in longer sentences.[23]

Aid to cognitive development

Shore and other researchers believe that baby talk contributes to mental development as it helps teach the child the basic function and structure of language.[10] Studies have found that responding to an infant's babble with meaningless babble aids the infant's development; while the babble has no logical meaning, the verbal interaction demonstrates to the child the bidirectional nature of speech, and the importance of verbal feedback. Some experts advise that parents should not talk to infants and young children solely in baby talk, but should integrate some normal adult speech as well. The high-pitched sound of infant-directed speech gives it special acoustic qualities which may appeal to the infant.[24] Infant-directed speech may aid a child in the acquisition and/or comprehension of language-particular rules which are otherwise unpredictable;[24] an example is the reduction or avoidance of pronoun reversal errors.[25] It has been also suggested that motherese is crucial for children to acquire the ability to ask questions.[26]

Universality and differences by region

Researchers Bryant and Barrett (2007)[27] have suggested (as have others before them, e.g., Fernald, 1992[28]) that baby talk exists universally across all cultures and is a species-specific adaptation.[29] Other researchers contend that it is not universal among the world's cultures, and argue that its role in helping children learn grammar has been overestimated. For evidence to back up their claims they point out that in some societies (such as certain Samoan tribes),[10] adults do not speak to their children at all until the children reach a certain age. Furthermore, even where baby-talk is used, it has many complicated grammatical constructions, and mispronounced or non-standard words. Other evidence suggests that baby talk is not a universal phenomenon: for example Schieffelin & Ochs (1983) describe the Kaluli tribe of Papua New Guinea who do not typically employ infant-directed speech.[30] Language acquisition in Kaluli children was not found to be significantly impaired. In other societies, it is more common to speak to children as one would to an adult, but with simplifications in grammar and vocabulary, with the belief that it will help them learn words as they are known in the standard form.

In order to relate to the child during baby talk, a parent may deliberately slur or fabricate some words, and may pepper the speech with nonverbal utterances. A parent might refer only to objects and events in the immediate vicinity, and will often repeat the child's utterances back to them. Since children employ a wide variety of phonological and morphological simplifications (usually distance assimilation or reduplication) in learning speech, such interaction results in the "classic" baby-words like na-na for grandmother, wawa for water, or din-din for dinner, where the child seizes on a stressed syllable of the input, and simply repeats it to form a word.

Although child directed speech proves to be a concept recognized cross-culturally, the extent to which caregivers rely on and use this method of communication differs based on cultural differences. Mothers in regions that display predominately introverted cultures are less likely to display a great deal of child directed speech, although it is still utilized. Further, the personality of each child experiencing child directed speech from a caregiver deeply impacts the extent to which a caregiver will use this method of communication.[14] Infant directed speech has been seen in a wide variety of languages such as Japanese, Italian, Mandarin, British English, American English, French, and German [31] This illustrates the universality of the practice and serves as an indication that infant directed speech is a necessary aspect of social development for children.[17] Although it is found in many cultures it is far from universal in terms of style and amount it is used.[32] A factor found to influence the way adults communicate with children is the way the culture views children. For example, if they view children as helpless and unable to understand, adults tend to interact with children less than if they believe that children are capable of learning and understanding. Often cultures lacking a form of CDS make up for it in other ways, such as involving the children more in everyday activities.[33]

Research suggests that in a tonal language, such as Mandarin, the use of child directed speech is beneficial to development. Specifically, Mandarin features lexical tones that must be used with every syllable which in turn convey meaning. It has been seen that in tonal languages such as in this example mothers use child directed speech by heightening the pitch of speaking making the product easier for infants to understand. The raising of pitch in speech associated with child directed speech is present cross culturally and occurs regardless of the language being spoken. Psychoacoustic studies on intonation have been used to further determine the effect of higher pitch and exaggerated syllables used in child directed speech. These tests have determined that the properties of child directed speech do not create additional difficulty for infants when attempting to distinguish speech. Instead, the raised pitch and elongated style of child directed speech allow for more effective communication. Further, Mandarin-speaking mothers who emphasized changes between phonemes had children with higher successes in language discrimination tests. These findings also occur cross culturally, and present themselves clearly in the English language. Ultimately, regardless of geographic region, the use of child directed speech allows infants to develop the capacity for language more quickly due to its distinct qualities.

Use with non-infants

The use of baby talk is not limited to interactions between adults and infants, as it may be used among adults, or by people to animals. In these instances, the outward style of the language may be that of baby talk, but is not considered actual "parentese", as it serves a different linguistic function (see pragmatics).

Patronizing / derogatory baby talk

Baby talk and imitations of it may be used by one non-infant to another as a form of verbal abuse, in which the talk is intended to infantilize the victim. This can occur during bullying, when the bully uses baby talk to assert that the victim is weak, cowardly, overemotional, or otherwise inferior.

Flirtatious baby talk

Baby talk may be used as a form of flirtation between sexual or romantic partners. In this instance, the baby talk may be an expression of tender intimacy, and may perhaps form part of affectionate sexual roleplaying in which one partner speaks and behaves childishly, while the other acts motherly or fatherly, responding in "parentese". One or both partners might perform the child role. Terms of endearment, such as poppet (or, indicatively, baby), may be used for the same purpose in communication between the partners.

Baby talk with pets

Surveys have indicated that many people speak to their dogs in their native language with more than just commands - they speak to them as if they were a child or another human being. Most people greet their dog when they come home and farewell them when they leave, or compliment them by telling them that they are pretty or clever. Many people admitted to often telling their dog about some sort of behavior and that it was stupid, naughty, helpful or fun. Sometimes this is extended to a short narrative, such as "It's a good thing I found this mess and tidied it up before your mother did". Most people admitted asking their dog questions about matters of the dog's concern, such as "Do you want to go for a walk?" or "Do you want a snack?". The majority of dog owners have asked their dog questions that they could not be expected to understand or care about, such as "Do you think it is going to rain today?". Some participate in a monologue, where they maintain a conversation but pause and look at the dog where the dog might be expected to make a comment. Others go further, and provide answers on behalf of the dog which may appear strange to outsiders. These actions are not providing communication with the dog, however they are providing social interactions for the speaker usually in order to solve some problem.[34]:304–306

As previously mentioned, baby talk (or Motherese) is a special kind of language that psychologists have found mothers use when talking to children. It is a simplified language spoken in a sing-song rhythm with lots of repetition, usually in a higher pitched tone of voice. The language people use when talking to dogs is very similar to Motherese and is referred to as Doggerel.[35] People tend to use sentences of around 11 words when talking to another adult but this is reduced to 4 words when talking to a dog. People speak more about imperatives or commands to a dog but they ask twice as many questions of the dog than they do other humans, even though they don't expect the dog to answer. Recordings show that 90% of Doggerel is spoken mostly in the present tense because people talk to dogs about what is happening now rather than the past or the future, which is twice as much as they do with humans. Also, people are 20 times more likely to repeat or rephrase themselves to dogs than they do to humans. All of these characteristics are similar between Motherese and Doggerel. The difference is that Motherese contains sentences about specific bits of information, such as "This cup is red". Motherese contains many more sentences of this sort because they are used to teach children about language and the environment. Doggerel only contains half of these sort of statements. Doggerel is serving a social function for humans; whether the dog learns anything does not seem to be a concern.[34]:308–310

In speaking Doggerel people use more than a higher tone, they also strongly emphasize intonations and any emotional phrasing. They use diminutives such as "walkie" for walk and "bathie" for bath. Words and phrases may be modified to make them less formal, using words such as "wanna" and "gonna". When people hear someone speak in a high-pitched sing-song voice "Do ya wanna have a snackie?", they can safely infer that they are talking to a dog, although there is a slight chance that they may be talking to a very young child. Although there is no evidence that speaking to a dog in Doggerel helps the dog understand what people are saying, there is a lot of evidence which suggests that talking to dogs in a normal, purposeful, and meaningful manner improves their receptive language abilities.[34]:310

Foreigner talk

When addressing a listener not skilled in the speaker's language, people may simplify their spoken language in an attempt to improve understanding. Some use sign language to communicate with others, especially if they have a hearing problem, although this is not always understood by people, as some signs in ASL may be difficult to interpret by some people, especially if gestures have different meanings from place to place, so they may use a baby talk-like language to communicate, skipping out small words and possibly using demonstratives instead of pronouns, for example Do not cross the road becoming No cross road. While this kind of simplifications could be helpful for say foreign tourists, this type of communication is perceived as rude or offensive in some societies, because it may cause the foreigner to feel infantilized. It can also be considered insulting if the foreigner is skilled in the speaker's language. While not considered to be actual parentese, it has aspects which make the two language styles similar.

Baby-talk words taken into adult speech

Sometimes baby-talk words are taken into adult speech. Examples are:

Vocabulary and structure

Vocabulary

With respect to English-speaking parents, it is well-established[36] that Anglo-Saxon or Germanic words tend to predominate in informal speech registers, whereas Latinate vocabulary is usually reserved for more formal uses such as legal and scientific texts. Child-directed speech, an informal speech register, also tends to use Anglo-Saxon vocabulary. The speech of mothers to young children has a higher percentage of native Anglo-Saxon verb tokens than speech addressed to adults.[37] In particular, in parents’ child-directed speech the clausal core[38] is built in the most part by Anglo-Saxon verbs, namely, almost all tokens of the grammatical relations subject-verb, verb-direct object and verb-indirect object that young children are presented with, are constructed with native verbs.[39] The Anglo-Saxon verb vocabulary consists of short verbs, but its grammar is relatively complex. Syntactic patterns specific to this sub-vocabulary in present-day English include periphrastic constructions for tense, aspect, questioning and negation, and phrasal lexemes functioning as complex predicates, all of which occur also in child-directed speech.

As noted above, baby talk often involves shortening and simplifying words, with the possible addition of slurred words and nonverbal utterances, and can invoke a vocabulary of its own. Some utterances are invented by parents within a particular family unit, or are passed down from parent to parent over generations, while others are quite widely known and used within most families, such as wawa for water, num-num for a meal, ba-ba for bottle, or beddy-bye for bedtime, and are considered standard or traditional words, possibly differing in meaning from place to place.

Baby talk, language regardless, usually consists of a muddle of words, including names for family members, names for animals, eating and meals, bodily functions and genitals, sleeping, pain, possibly including important objects such as diaper, blanket, pacifier, bottle, etc., and may be sprinkled with nonverbal utterances, such as goo goo ga ga. The vocabulary of made-up words, such as those listed below, may be quite long with terms for a large number of things, rarely or possibly never using proper language, other times quite short, dominated by real words, all nouns. Most words invented by parents have a logical meaning, although the nonverbal sounds are usually completely meaningless and just fit the speech together.

A fair number of baby talk and nursery words refer to bodily functions or the genitals, partly because the words are relatively easy to pronounce. Also, if a child is very young, bodily functions such as urination and defecation may be quite exciting for them. Scientific terms may be harder for them to understand and pronounce, so baby talk may be more convenient for a young child. Moreover, such words reduce adults' discomfort with the subject matter, and make it possible for children to discuss such things without breaking adult taboos. However, some, such as pee-pee and poo-poo have been very widely used in reference to bodily functions to the point that they are considered to be standard words, so ability to mention such subjects without adult negativity has recently faded.

Sometimes baby talk words escape from the nursery and get into adult vocabulary, for example "nanny" for "children's nurse" or "nursery governess".[40]

Diminutives

Moreover, many words can be derived into baby talk following certain rules of transformation, in English adding a terminal /i/ sound at the end, usually written and spelled as /ie/, /y/, or /ey/, is a common way to form a diminutive which is often used as part of baby talk, examples include:

("Puppy" is often erroneously thought to be a diminutive of pup made this way, but it is in fact the other way around: pup is a shortening of puppy, which comes from French popi or poupée which means "doll". "Doll" is such a shortening for "dolly", which started as a pet form for "Dorothy".)

Phrases

Baby talk phrases and sentences often skip out small words, imitating young children who can make little sense of sentence composition, such as to, at, for, my, so and as, and articles (the, a, an), thus resulting in an incomplete sentence, such as I need go potty or I want blanket. Sometimes, demonstratives are used instead of pronouns (he, I, it, she etc.), as it may help children learn people's names, for example, Daddy wants Susie to eat her cereal instead of standard adult-type speech, I want you to eat your cereal as pronouns are often confusing to young children. Also, labelling is practised, sometimes emphasising a word through repetition within a sentence, such as That's a car, Susie. It's a car. Some parents substitute a particular word in a sentence with a difficult sound to pronounce with another easier word, such as choo-choo instead of train as some children are unable to pronounce the /tr/ sound as infants, although most learn pronunciations and phonics as they increase in age.

All individual words have a logical meaning, although phrases made up of them are often based on random utterances, sprinkled with logical words, so the child can "sift" out the words with meanings and interpret them, as the parent may teach language by labelling, associating the word with the object or action.

Use as informal terms

Baby talk words and phrases such as mama, pee-pee, potty, yucky, no-no and tummy are sometimes used after infancy as colloquial or informal terms. However, reduplication is not practiced. For example, pee-pee becomes pee. Also, meanings may alter slightly to become more age-universal and specific: for example potty changing in meaning from any toilet to a container-like one for small children, yum-yum changing from mealtime to an informal expression of delight towards a meal, or stinky changing from defecation (as a countable noun) to an adjective for something smelling bad. Poppet or similar terms may be used as a term of endearment for a loved one of similar age, such as a romantic partner, and quick-quick or no-no may be used as expressions in school, university and even occupational work scenarios. Nonverbal utterances such as googoogaga may be used as figuratives for things misinterpreted or not understood. Words such as mama and nana are words often used for family members past infancy. The word doo-doo is used as a figurative later in age for something difficult or problematic, such as When the computer's jammed, we're in deep doo-doo.

Duplications

Most standard baby talk words consist of a single syllable duplicated, such as mama, dada, poo-poo, pee-pee, baba, boo-boo, bot-bot, num-num, dum-dum and wee-wee. These are often imitations of a baby's first utterances which take the shape of a word. These are made when the child takes a stressed syllable of the main word to shorten it and repeats it to form a word-like utterance. Words with similar sounds from stressed syllables, such as mama, dada and babainclude:

Words can be made from a diminutive with an /i/ sound at the end, reduplicated, but the first letter of the duplication replaced with a /w/- for example teensy-weensy, puppy-wuppy or binkie-winkie. Realistic language examples following this same pattern known as partial duplications or modified duplications include okey-dokey, silly-billy, mumbo-jumbo and super-duper, which use the first letter as the point of modification. However, these patterns are more common in colloquial language and slang than formal English and rarely use a /w/ for modification.

Many baby talk words for animals involve duplication of the onomatopoeia of the sound they make, including:

Others, which do not relate to animals, include vroom-vroom (car) and choo-choo (train).

Differences in pronunciation

Other transformations mimic the way infants mistake certain consonants which in English can include turning /l/ into /w/ as in wuv from love or widdo from little or in pronouncing /v/ as /b/ and /ð/ or /t/ as /d/ and /θ/ as /f/ or /s/. It is a way of imitating how a baby or young child speaks, because most babies, when they start talking, talk as though they have a speech disorder because they mistake certain consonants. This usually is outgrown by the age of five or six.

Still other transformations, but not in all languages, include elongated vowels, such as kitty and kiiiitty, (emphasized /i/) meaning the same thing. While this is understood by English speaking toddlers, it is not applicable with Dutch toddlers as they learn that elongated vowels reference different words.[41]

Phonology

Infant directed speech includes features of higher pitch, exaggerated pitch changes, elongated vowels and long pauses between phonemes. This is done in order to allow infants time to process the information being conveyed to them. Rhythm is also heavily emphasized in this practice and is used closely with the emphasis of various syllables. Vowel space is also expanded in infant directed speech allowing for accurate phoneme discrimination. Words that are somewhat difficult in terms of the sounds that make them up, or phonologically difficult words, might be simplified.

Syntax

Infant directed speech features a unique syntax, usually having a simplified form. Caregivers utilizing infant directed speech often use short utterances rather than full sentence structures in order to convey meaning to their infants. These short units are often repeated so infants have practice in a particular concept. Infant directed speech allows for infants to detect syntactic boundaries. Further, infant directed speech makes linguistic patterns easier to discover than when adult directed speech is used. Infants begin to understand word order through infant directed speech which slowly expands into a deeper understanding of sentence structure as a whole.

Communicating with children can be difficult if the adult cannot maintain their attention, so the topic must be on things that interest them. Research has found that five topics tend to dominate the conversation: members of the family, animals, parts of the body, food, and clothing. Conversations with children are mostly about the present and the here-and-now, rather than topics pertaining to another time, past or future.

Characteristics

Child Directed Speech (CDS) is a clear and simplified way of communicating to younger children used not only by adults but also by older children. The vocabulary is limited, speech is slowed with a greater number of pauses, and the sentences are short and grammatically simplified and often repeated.[33] Three types of modifications occur to adult directed speech in the production of infant directed speech. Firstly are linguistic modifications, including the simplification of speech units as well as emphasis on various phonemes. Secondly are modifications to attention getting strategies. Caregivers use visual movements of the face to more effectively gain and maintain the attention of their infants. Lastly, modifications are made to the interactions between parents and infants. Parents use infant directed speech not only to promote language development, but to create a bond and positive relationship between them and their infants.[17]

The younger the child the more exaggerated CDS is. And because it is slow, simple and easier to understand, research has proven that infants prefer CDS over normal speech.[42] Research shows that one of the most prominent characteristics of infant directed speech is the wider opening of the mouth present in those using infant directed speech versus adult directed speech. This occurs especially in vowels. Vocal pitch is also heightened during infant directed speech and research indicates vowels are spoken with the highest pitch when infant directed speech is used. The horizontal positioning of the lips in infant directed speech does not differ significantly from positioning used in adult directed speech. Instead, the only observed difference remains in vertical lip positioning. By making the opening of the lips larger, infants are more likely to focus on the face of the speaker. Research suggests that the larger the opening of the lips during infant directed speech, the better infants will understand the message being conveyed due to the additional visual cues available.[14] Contrary to popular belief, the intensity of speech, however, does not differ from that used in adult directed speech.

Infant directed speech not only includes the characteristics of high pitched speech and elongated syllables, but also incorporates visual body movements that assist in conveying meaning of language to infants. One monumental visual aspect of infant directed speech is the movement of the lips. In addition, head movements are used to emphasize various syllables within language production. These visual cues provide infants the additional information needed to perform accurate speech discrimination during language development. Visual cues also allow infants to discriminate speech differences in environments in which they cannot rely on their hearing, such as in noisy environments. However, the auditory and visual aspects of infant directed speech do not exist independently. Infants rely equally on both methods of understanding and as development continues, infants strengthen the link between these two important categories.

It must be considered that although infant directed speech features marked characteristics, it may not only be these characteristics aiding in development of language for children. Due to the visual cues used by caregivers in this method of communication, infants are more highly motivated to engage in communication. Because infants are willing to participate in this process, caregivers are able to make significant progress through the use of infant directed speech.[14]

It is often assumed that infants are interested in the properties of infant directed speech and play a passive role in this interaction. However, research indicates that infants are not only attracted to the practice itself but to people who engage in infant directed speech. Through this practice, infants are able to determine who positive and encouraging caregivers will be in their development. When infants use infant directed speech as a determinant of acceptable caregivers their cognitive development seems to thrive because they are being encouraged by adults who are invested in the development of the given infants.[18]

Examples in literature

...pressing her cheek to Flopit's, she changed her tone. "Izzum's ickle heart a-beatin' so floppity! Um's own mumsy make ums all right, um's p'eshus Flopit!"
"A Peke, the ickle angel pet, wiv his gweat big soulful eyes and his ickle black nosie — oh so ducky-duck!"

See also

References

  1. Matychuk, Paul (24 April 2004). "The role of child-directed speech in language acquisition: a case study" (PDF). Language Sciences (27).
  2. Berey, Adam (March 18, 2005). "Gender Differences in Child-Directed Speech". Lawrence University.
  3. Herrera, E.; Reissland, N.; Shepherd, J. (2004). "Maternal touch and maternal child-directed speech : effects of depressed mood in the postnatal period". Journal of Affective Disorders. 81 (1): 29–39. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2003.07.001. PMID 15183597.
  4. Ghada Khattab. "Does child-directed speech really facilitate the emergence of phonological structure? The case of gemination in Arabic CDS" (PDF). Archived from the original (PDF) on November 27, 2006.
  5. Pinker on Motherese - http://courses.education.illinois.edu/edpsy313/notes/pinker_motherese.html
  6. Fernald, Anne. "Four-Month-Old Infants Prefer to Listen to Motherese". Infant Behavior and Development. 8.
  7. Newport, E.L.; Gleitman, L. (1977). C.E. Snow & C.A. Ferguson, ed. Talking to children: Language input and acquisition. pp. 109–150.
  8. Lawrence Balter; Robert B. McCall. Parenthood in America: A-M. ABC-CLIO. p. 75. ISBN 978-1-57607-213-4. developmental psychologists refer to this kind of language to young children as child-directed speech
  9. Kathy L. Reschke, Ph.D.(2002), Ohio State University, "Baby Talk" Archived August 22, 2007, at the Wayback Machine. Archived copy at the Library of Congress (November 22, 2006).
  10. 1 2 3 Shore, Rima. (1997). Rethinking the brain: New insights into early development. New York: Families and Work Institute.
  11. Baby Talk May Help Infants Learn Faster
  12. Singh, Leher; Nestor, Sarah; Parikh, Chandni; Yull, Ashley (2009). "Influences of infant-directed speech on early word recognition". Psychology Press. 14 (6). doi:10.1080/15250000903263973.
  13. revised from Lindon, J (2005) Understanding Child Development - Linking Theories and Practice, Hodder Arnold, London. Added by S M Burnett, Scotland
  14. 1 2 3 4 Green, Jordan; Nip,Ignatius; Wilson,Erin; Mefferd,Antje; Yunusova,Yana (December 2010). "Lip movement exaggerations during infant-directed speech". Journal of Speech, Language & Hearing Research. 53 (6): 1529–1542. doi:10.1044/1092-4388(2010/09-0005).
  15. Singh, Leher; Best, Catherine; Morgan, James (2003). "Infants' Listening Preferences: Baby Talk or Happy Talk?". Infancy. 3 (3): 365–395. doi:10.1207/s15327078in0303_5.
  16. Waterson, N (1978). The development of communication. Chichester, UK: Wiley. pp. 199–216.
  17. 1 2 3 Mcleod, Peter (July 1993). "What studies of communication with infants ask us about psychology: Baby-talk and other speech registers". Canadian Psychology. 34 (3). doi:10.1037/h0078828.
  18. 1 2 Schachner, Adena; Hannon, Erin (January 2011). "Infant-directed speech drives social preferences in 5-month-old infants". Developmental Psychology. 47 (1): 19–25. doi:10.1037/a0020740.
  19. Kaplan, Peter; Jung, Paula; Ryther, Jennifer; Zarlengo-Strouse, Patricia (September 1996). "Infant-directed versus adult-directed speech as signals for face". Developmental Psychology. 32 (5): 880–891. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.32.5.880.
  20. Singh, Leher; Nestor, Sarah; Parikh, Chandni; Yull, Ashley (November–December 2009). "Influences of Infant-Directed Speech on Early Word Recognition". Infancy. 14 (6): 654–666. doi:10.1080/15250000903263973.
  21. Goldstein, M.H., and J.A. Schwade. "Social Feedback to Infants’ Babbling Facilitates Rapid Phonological Learning." Psychological Science. 19.5 (2008): 515-523. Web.
  22. Kaplan, Peter; Dungan, Jessica; Zinser, Michael (March 2004). "Infants of Chronically Depressed Mothers Learn in Response to Male, But Not Female, Infant-Directed Speech". Developmental Psychology. 40 (2): 140–148. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.40.2.140.
  23. Hoff, E. (2003). "The specificity of environmental influence: Socioeconomic status affects early vocabulary development via maternal speech". Child Development. 74: 1368–1378. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00612.
  24. 1 2 (Goodluck 1991)
  25. Kaznatcheev, Artem (2010). "A Connectionist Study on the Interplay of Nouns and Pronouns in Personal Pronoun Acquisition". Cognitive Computation. 2 (4): 280–284. doi:10.1007/s12559-010-9050-7.
  26. Joseph Jordania (2006). Who Asked the First Question? The Origins of Human Choral Singing, Intelligence, Language and Speech. Tbilisi: Logos. ISBN 99940-31-81-3.
  27. Bryant, G. A. and Barrett, H. C. (2007). Recognizing intentions in infant-directed speech: Evidence for universals. Psychological Science, 18(8), 746-751. http://gabryant.bol.ucla.edu/research.html
  28. Fernald, A. (1992). "Human maternal vocalizations to infants as biologically relevant signals". In J. Barkow, L. Cosmides, & J. Tooby (Eds.), The adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture (pp. 391–428). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
  29. Grieser, DiAnne; Patricia K. Kuhl (January 1988). "Maternal Speech to Infants in a Tonal Language: Support for Universal Prosodic Features in Motherese". Developmental Psychology. 24 (1): 14–20. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.24.1.14.
  30. Ochs, Elinor and Bambi Schieffelin. (1984). "Language acquisition and socialization: Three developmental stories". Culture Theory Eds. R. Shweder and R. LeVine. pp. 276–320.
  31. Cooper, Robin Panneton; Aslin, Richard N. (1990-01-01). "Preference for Infant-Directed Speech in the First Month after Birth". Child Development. 61 (5): 1584–1595. doi:10.2307/1130766. JSTOR 1130766.
  32. Gallaway, C. Input and interaction in language acquisition. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. pp. 56–73.
  33. 1 2 Harley, Trevor (2010). Talking the Talk: Language, Psychology and Science. Psychology Press. pp. 60–62. ISBN 978-1-84169-339-2.
  34. 1 2 3 Coren, Stanley "How To Speak Dog: Mastering the Art of Dog-Human Communication" 2000 Simon & Schuster, New York.
  35. Hirsh-Pasek, Kathy; Treiman, Rebecca (2008). "Doggerel: Motherese in a new context". Journal of Child Language. 9. doi:10.1017/S0305000900003731.
  36. Quirk, R. (1974). The linguist and the English language. London: Arnold. (p. 138).
  37. Phillips, J. (1973). Syntax and vocabulary of mothers' speech to young children: Age and sex comparisons. Child Development, 44, 182-185.
  38. Foley, W. A. and Van Valin, R. D. Jr. (1984). Functional syntax and universal grammar. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  39. Ninio, A. (2011). Syntactic development, its input and output. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Introduction accessible at http://fds.oup.com/www.oup.com/pdf/13/9780199565962_prelim.pdf
  40. Travers, P.L. (1934). Mary Poppins. Houghton Mifflin Harcourt.
  41. "Native Language Governs Toddlers' Speech Sounds". therapytimes.com. 2007-10-23. Retrieved 2007-11-02.
  42. Fernald, A. (1991). "Prosody and focus in speech to infants and adults". Annals of Child Development. 8: 43–80.

Further reading

External links

This article is issued from Wikipedia - version of the 11/15/2016. The text is available under the Creative Commons Attribution/Share Alike but additional terms may apply for the media files.